DIAGNOSTIC OF JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS’
MICROCOSMIC
PARTICLE CONCEPTIONS OF PURE SUBSTANCE AND AMALGAM
Rong
-
Chun Chang
, National Changhua University of Education,
Changhus
,
Taiwan
Huey
-
Por Chang, National Changhua University of Education,
Changhus
,
Tai
wan
Tzu
-
Shan Cheng,
Taipei Municipal Minzu Elementary School
&
National Changhua
University of Education,
Taipei & Changhus
,
Taiwan
The purpose of this study was to explore students’
understanding of
microcosmic particle
conceptions
and proof the
consiste
nce
of explanation to application questions
in pure
substance and amalgam.
we used
the questionnaire,
interviews
and pictures which the
students drew
to evaluate
thirty
-
seven
students’
ideas
in the grade
-
8.
The results indicated
that
:
(a)
The
students’
micr
ocosmic particle
conceptions
about pure substance and amalgam
could be classified into three types.
(b)
Most students
classified
the matters as pure substance
or amalgam by
the spontaneous description definition.
(c)
Very few students could apply the
micro
cosmic particle
conceptions
to explain the advanced questions.
DIAGNOSTIC OF JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS’
MICROCOSMIC
PARTICLE CONCEPTIONS OF PURE SUBSTANCE AND AMALGAM
Objectives
The scientific
concept
learning
to many students is difficult.
Engel Clou
gh and Driver
(1986) reported that students were using different alternative frameworks in response to
parallel questions. Thus, it appears that in many cases, students do not apply their conceptions
in a way that a scientist would consider to be consisten
t.
Reif (1987) concluded that novice
students' knowledge about a scientific concept is highly fragmented and does not specify how
to interpret a concept in specific instances.
Most people who hold misconceptions are not
aware that their ideas are incorrect
. Possessing misconceptions can have serious impacts on
our learning.
The purpose of this study was to explore junior high school students’
developing
understanding of microcosmic particle
conceptions
by examining
students’
cognition of pure
substance and
amalgam. Through the definition of the concepts, using their ideas to class with
ten different matters and drawing pictures, the students
’
conceptions would be confirmed and
accordable.
The results
of this study
would supply
accurate
information
about stud
ents
’
understanding of
pure substance and amalgam. Further, we examined the consistence of
students
’
thoughts between understanding and application.
Further,
we
investigate
d the
consistence with students
’
explanation between understanding and application
,
and our
research questions were as follows: (1)What
do
the
student
s understand about microcosmic
particle
conceptions
of pure substance and amalgam?
(2)
What do the student
s express
the
concept image knowledge
of pure substance and amalgam?
(3)
How are
the co
nsistence of
students
’
conceptions about
pure substance and amalgam
between understanding and
application?
Significance
This study provided a detailed description of these students
’
ideas about the
microcosmic
particle
conceptions
of pure substance and ama
lgam. We studied not only the content and the
class
of the students
’
ideas but also examined whether the students
’
ideas were coherent
between understanding and application. The results of this study provide important
information about the class of student
s
’
conceptual understanding and concretely present
students
’
image knowledge by drawing pictures. Ultimately, this research will help teachers
develop appropriate instructional strategies to facilitate student learning.
Theory
1.Theoretical Perspective
Sci
entific explanations incorporate existing scientific knowledge and new evidence from
observations, experiments, or models into internally consistent, logical statements. Different
terms, such as "hypothesis," "model," "law," "principle," "theory," and "par
adigm" are used to
describe various types of scientific explanations. As students develop and as they understand
more science concepts and processes, their explanations should become more sophisticated.
That is, their scientific explanations should more fr
equently include a rich scientific
knowledge base, evidence of logic, higher levels of analysis, greater tolerance of criticism and
uncertainty, and a clearer demonstration of the relationship between logic, evidence, and
current knowledge(NRC, 1996). Stud
ents' constructed knowledge typically has two properties:
it can be incorrect, and it can often impede the learning of conventionally accepted knowledge
(Chi & Roscoe, 2002).
Many research assumed that people spontaneously generate ideas to account for
phe
nomena in the natural world(Carey,
1991
;
Driver
&
Easley, 1978
;
McCloskey
&
Kargon,1988
;
Osborne
&
Wittrock,
1983).
These
spontaneously ideas, although often
inaccurate from the perspective of scientific theory, help people organize and explain events
a
round them (
McCloskey
&
Kargon, 1988
;Vosniadou
&
Brewer,1992
;Wiser,1988)
.
The
middle school students could not be classified as having consistent
knowledge
frameworks
because their ideas were very fragmented. The fragmentation of middle school students
’
ide
as
about matter probably reflects the difficulty of assimilating the microscopic level scientific
knowledge acquired through formal instruction into students
’
initial macroscopic knowledge
frameworks
(Nakhleh, Samarapungavan,
&
Saglam, 2005)
.
2.Prior Resea
rch
Most of the middle school students interviewed knew that matter was composed of
atoms and molecules and some of them were able to use this knowledge to explain some
processes such as phase transitions of water. In contrast, almost no elementary studen
ts knew
that matter was composed of atoms and molecules. However, the middle school students were
unable to consistently explain material properties or processes based on their knowledge of
material composition(Nakhleh, Samarapungavan,
&
Saglam, 2005)
.
Gar
nett, Garnett and
Hackling (1995) indicate that it is difficult for introductory chemistry students to develop
adequate conceptions of the unobservable entities (atoms and molecules) and events involved
in chemical reactions.
Thomas and McRobbie (2002) foun
d that secondary school students
frequently explain material phenomena at a macroscopic rather than a microscopic level.
Reviews of
microcosmic particle
conceptions research indicate that it is difficult to develop
for students
(Abraham, Grzybowski, Renner
&
Marek, 1992
;
Benson, Wittrock
;
Baur, 1993
;
Kokkotas & Vlachos, 1998
).
Because of
microcosmic particle
conceptions are abstract model
theory.
However, we have found no in
-
depth studies that specifically examine middle school
students’ cognitive level
of
microcosmic particle
conceptions
about pure substance and
amalgam
.
Design and Procedure
1.Sample Description
We conducted the present study with eighth graders at an suburban middle school in the
South Taiwan in the fall of 2005. The author interviewed
a class of students including
thirty
-
eight persons. This class students’ academic achievement presents the habit
distribution.
2.Methodology
The questionnaire
and semistructured interview guide are two major tools in this study.
The questionnaire which t
he
authors designed was implemented into two steps
:
First,
investigate
students’
understanding
in definition, property
and
composition of pure substance
and amalgam. In order to assist in explanation, s
tudents
were asked to give examples and
draw pictures
;
Second, according to the conceptions of pure substa
nce and amalgam, ask
s
tudents
to classify ten different matters. In advance,
students
explain and draw pictures to
answer the questions about matters
’
shape and composition. After analyzing the data gathered
from the students
’
answers to the questionnaire,
the author employed semi
-
structured
individual interviews with thirty
-
seven
students in the grade
-
8 to confirm
students’
ideas.
Findings
1.
The
students’
microcosmic particle
conceptions
about pure substance and amalgam
could be
classified into three types
:
the spontaneous description definition, the matter
’
s property
definition, and the microcosmic particle
conceptions
definition. Most students(48.6
%
) define
pure substance and amalgam
by the matter
’
s property. The spontaneous description definition
is secon
dary(43.2
%).
Very few students(8.1
%
) have microcosmic particle
conceptions
in
definition.The first type students drew
pure substance
which were composed of the same
particle
s and
amalgam
which were composed of above two kinds different
particle
s. It means
that the students have the conceptions about matters are composed of small particles.
However, the
microcosmic particle
conceptions are a kind of model theory. The students’
observation methods just like using a magnifying glass. It expressed that the stud
ents didn’t
have
the microcosmic particle
conceptions.
The second type students
’ ideas about
pure
substance and amalgam
were very fragmented. These ideas were affected by school teaching.
Through the school teaching activities, the students learned about t
he boiling point of pure
substance
is fixed, but it is not certainly fixed of
amalgam
. Furthermore, the students thought
the boiling point of all kinds of pure
substance
is
100
0
C. If the matter’s boiling point is not
100
0
C
,
the matter must be a kind of
ama
lgam
. Only three students are the third type
(8.1
%
)
.
They could use
microcosmic particle
conceptions
to explain the
words of
pure substance and
amalgam
. In addition, the chemistry nouns of "element" and "compound" were used in
interview.
2.
Most students(89.
1
%
)
classified
the matters as pure substance or amalgam by
the
spontaneous description definition. There are 34.2
%
students based on the meaning of the
words and 45.9
%
students considered the composition of the matter. Very few students(10.8
%
)
use the micr
ocosmic particle
conceptions
and nobody(0
%
)
applied the matter
’
s property.
On the whole, the students
classified
the
solid
matters as pure substance or amalgam making
more mistakes
(
the copper metal 56.8%, the carbon 16.2% and the glucose 45.9%
). Most
stud
ents thought
the carbon
is a kind of amalgam not pure substance. The interview data
asserted that the students are
more familiar
with the matters
is easier to have many ideas
.
However
, these ideas
are uncertain
correct. The students
classified
the
liqui
d matter
s as pure
substance or amalgam
(
the water 75.7%, the cement 97.3%, the soft drink 97.3% and the
salt
water 94.6%)
more correct.
The student indicated that the water and the salt water are
often
used
in teaching activity
and
the soft drink and the c
ement are
frequently contacted in daily
life
.
The students
classified
the
gas
matters as pure substance or amalgam
had the
contradictory situation
. Many students thought
carbon dioxide is a kind of pure matter
(67.6%), but only 56.8%
students
thought
the a
ir is
amalgam
.
It means that there are some
students
who
thought in the air do not include the carbon dioxide
.
Although
the
students had
studied the
conceptions about the
air in the elementary
school , but the research
asserted
that
many students
still
do
not understand the composition
of the
ai
r
.
In spite of the
student
s
could
give oxygen, hydrogen and carbon dioxide
for example
s to
expla
i
n
the air but they could not
think the air is a kind of amalgam
.
3.
Very few students could apply the microcosmic parti
cle
conceptions
to explain the advanced
questions about the composition, shape and phase transition of matter. Most students(72.9
%
)
still keep macroscopic knowledge frameworks.
Minority student
s(16.2
%
) can
propose
the
conceptions about
atom or member
.
Eve
n though
the students used
atom or member
to
explain questions, their pictures still presented macroscopic knowledge frameworks. Very few
students(10.8
%
)
understand the
microcosmic particle
theory. There are four students have the
knowledge of
the
microcos
mic particle
conceptions and also use the ideas to explain
questions about the material shape for example solid, liquid and gas. It asserted that the
students have coherent cognition about
the
microcosmic particle
conceptions between
between understanding
and application.
4.The result
s of this study can assist science teachers to understand students
’
learning about
the microcosmic particle a
bstract
conceptions
and provide reference resources for designing
instructional materials and methods to help students
constructing scientific
conceptions
.
References
Abraham, M. R., Grzybowski, E. B., Renner, J. W.
&
Marek, E. A. (1992). Understandings
and misunderstandings of eighth graders of five chemistry concepts found in textbooks.
Journal of Research in Science T
eaching,
29
(2)
,
105
-
120
.
Benson, D. L., Wittrock, M. C.,
&
Baur, M. E. (1993).
Students
’
preconceptions of the nature
of gases.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
30
(9)
,
1169
-
1187
.
Carey,
S. (
1991
). Knoweldge acquisition
:
Enrichment or conceptual chan
ge? In S. Carey
&
R.
Gelman (Eds), The epigenesis of mind:Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257
-
292).
Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.
Chi, M. T. H.,
&
Roscoe, R. D. (2002). The process and challenges of conceptual change. In
M. Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), "Reconsider
ing conceptual change: Issues in theory and
practice" (pp. 3
-
27). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Driver, R.
&
Easley, J. (1978). Pupils and paradigms
:
A review of the literature related to
concept development in adolescent science students.
Studies in Science Education
, 10
,
37
-
60.
Engel Clough, E., & Driver, R. (1986). A study of consistency in the use of students'
conceptual frameworks across different task contexts.
Science Education, 70
(4),
473
-
496.
Garnett, P. J., Garnett, P. J.,
&
Hackling M. W. (1995). Students’ a
lternative conceptions in
chemistry: A review of research and implications for teaching and learning.
Studies in
Science Education, 25
, 69
-
95.
Kokkotas, P.,
&
Vlachos, I. (1998). Teaching
the topic of the particulate nature of matter in
prospective teacher
s' training courses.
International
Journal of Science Education,
20
(3),
291
-
303.
McCloskey, M.
&
Kargon, R. (1988). The meaning and use of historical models in the study
of intuitive physics. In S. Strauss (Ed.), Ontogeny, phylogeny and historical developm
e
nt
(pp. 49
-
67). Norwood, NJ
:
Ablex.
National Research Council. (1996). The National Science Education Standards. Washington
DC: National Academy Press.
Nakhleh, M.B., Samarapungavan, A.
&
Saglam, Y. (2005). Middle school students
’
beliefs
about matter. Jour
nal of Research in Science Teaching, 42, 581
-
612.
Osborne, R.J.
&
Wittrock, M.C. (1983). Learning science
:
A generative process. Science
Education, 67, 489
-
508.
Reif, F. (1987). Instructional design, cognition and technology: Applications to the teaching of
scientific concepts.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24
(4), 309
-
324.
Thomas, G.P.
&
McRobbie, C.J. (2002). Collaborating to enhance student reasoning:Frances’
account of her reflections while teaching chemical equilibrium. International Journal o
f
Science Education, 24, 405
-
423.
Vosniadou, S.
&
Brewer, W.F. (1992). Mental models of the earth
:
A study of conceptual
change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535
-
585.
Wiser, M. (1988). The differentiation of heat and temperature
:
History of science
and novice
expert shift. In S. Strauss (Ed.),
Ontogeny, phylogeny and historical development (pp.
28
-
48). Norwood, NJ
:
Ablex.
Enter the password to open this PDF file:
File name:
-
File size:
-
Title:
-
Author:
-
Subject:
-
Keywords:
-
Creation Date:
-
Modification Date:
-
Creator:
-
PDF Producer:
-
PDF Version:
-
Page Count:
-
Preparing document for printing…
0%
Comments 0
Log in to post a comment